Deviance as an historical artefact: a scoping review of psychological studies of body modification (2024)

Introduction

Body modifications in general and tattooing in particular have increased in popularity steadily over the last 50 years in England, the United States, and other industrialised countries (Burns, 2019; DeMello, 2000; Statista Research Department, 2021). The culture around tattooing and other body modifications in the developed and developing countries has shifted rapidly; however, attitudes can be resistant to change. Moreover, while pictures of tattoos and other body modifications populate Instagram and other visually oriented social media platforms, a recent opinion piece from The Times that enjoyed a wide circulation during the writing of this article (“Seeing tattoos makes me feel physically sick: Ubiquity of body art is born out of an existential crisis of humanity in the post-religious world” by Melanie Phillips, 2022) supports the notion that mainstream attitudes may lag behind cultural portrayals.

There are numerous means to permanently modify the body (Table 1), so this is potentially a vast literature. The contemporary history of all body modifications is beyond the scope of this article, but we recommend interested readers consult Pitts-Taylor (2003), Eubanks (1996), Vale and Juno (1989) as but a few examples. However, the body modification literature focuses primarily on tattooing, so a short history at least of tattooing may be illustrative.

Tattooing and body modifications in general are likely as old as the human species based on multiple lines of circ*mstantial evidence. The oldest evidence of permanent modification comes from the teeth of a Late Pleistocene hominid from Olduvai Gorge in Tanzania, Africa (12–20 k years old) (Willman et al., 2020), while the tattooed mummy commonly known as Ötzi is currently the oldest definitive evidence of tattooing (Deter-Wolf et al., 2016). Tattooing has been practiced around the world and was well-known to Europeans before colonial navigators began exploring the world’s oceans but appears to have waned in popularity with the rise of nation-states (Buss and Hodges, 2017). Nevertheless, when Captain James Cook encountered tattooed peoples throughout the Pacific, the facial tattooing and patterns observed were so striking that the Polynesia word tatau (meaning, “to strike”) was picked up by Cook’s sailors to describe what they had seen and in some cases experienced (Douglas, 2005). Missionaries and other colonial agents suppressed tattooing as non-Christian in many parts of the world, but some tattooing traditions were maintained through this period and have resurged in popularity (Buss and Hodges, 2017; Caplan, 1997, 2000b; DeMello, 2000).

Tattooing in Europe and North America were popular as both exotic collections of the wealthy or the souvenirs of soldiers and sailors until the invention of the electric tattoo machine (Caplan, 1997; Lodder, 2013). In the nineteenth century, Thomas Edison’s newly invented electric pen was modified by Samuel O’Reilly in the United States to patent the first electric tattooing machine. The further development of tattoo machines prompted the appearance of tattoo parlours in major cities throughout Europe and North America, where tattoos became affordable for the working classes and extremely popular (Lodder, 2013). As tattooing flourished among the poor and penal populations swelled, especially among urban poor, attitudes toward tattooing shifted. Criminologists began collecting the skin of deceased tattooed people, which were used to categorise and diagnose tendencies toward criminality and mental illness (Angel, 2017; Lodder, 2013). Though multiple competing models of deviance were developed, the focus in the writings of nineteenth century criminologists on “the soldier, seaman, or ‘recidivist’” belies the fact that British royal military figureheads may have played a part in reinvigorating interest in pilgrimage and souvenir tattoos that persisted beyond the era (Angel, 2017).

The effort to taxonomically categorise criminal tendencies was a failure in terms of identifying deviance, but it may have helped create the stigma it sought to describe, along with associations being made between tattooing and lower social classes in Europe and North America (Bradley, 2000; Caplan, 2000a). The working-class popularity of tattooing waxed during the twentieth century with the two world wars, as military and non-military alike collected patriotic emblems, but popularity waned after the wars. The uniforms of working-class professions increased the visibility of tattoos, and such visible tattooing also became negatively associated with groups adjacent to the working class, like bikers and gang members (DeMello, 2000). These were not the first concerns and class, as fears about tattooing among minors and erotic designs appearing on women led to efforts to legally suppress tattooing in Germany in the 1910s and in the United States in the 1930s (Govenar, 2000; Oettermann, 2000). Negative associations with tattooing finally led to a complete ban in New York City in 1961 due to a purported concern over hepatitis transmission, which had a ripple effect across the United States that persisted until the ban was lifted in New York in 1997 (McCabe, 1997).

Pressure from city health departments all over North America led to innovations in tattooing by artists seeking to revitalise the industry. As a study of legal dynamics of tattooing in Vancouver, Canada makes clear, the pressures from health departments led to changes that, over the succeeding decades, have become local law (Jelinski, 2018). These innovations set the stage for what Rubin (1988) called the “second tattoo renaissance,” a resurgence in the popularity akin to the uptick after the electric tattoo machine was invented. As one of us (Lynn) observed first-hand, tattooing seemed to explode in popularity in New York City and throughout North America and Europe after New York City’s ban was lifted, though speculation and stigma about the psychology of modified individuals remains.

The mystery around tattooing and body modifications in general is not why people engage in them—the real enigma is how and why the symbolic importance of body modifications (or any cultural practice) changes societies over time or in response to sociocultural factors. Don Ed Hardy, one of the foremost tattoo artists of the second tattoo renaissance, points out that tattoos are windows into the psyche but also like “Geiger counters for people’s fears” (Vale and Juno, 1989, p. 51), telling more about the viewer of the tattoo than the wearer. Given social and technological changes that have fuelled the body modification renaissance of the past 50 years, it is important that the granularity of our research questions and designs follow similar evolutions.

As body modifications have increased in global popularity, so too have studies of body modifications proliferated in a way that is theoretically and methodologically diverse. Despite this, we were surprised to find recent studies that seemed to take antiquated theoretical perspectives by framing research in terms of correlations with risk behaviour (e.g., Broussard and Harton, 2018). Why do researchers continue to revisit the notion that people with body modifications represent or are from stigmatised groups despite the overwhelming number of studies indicating that body modifications more accurately reflect prosocial rather than anti-social means of social communication (examples of significant treatments include Atkinson, 2004; Lingel and Boyd, 2013; Pitts, 2003)? To address this question, we conducted a scoping review of the psychological literature on body modifications.

Methods

First, we established which body modifications were relevant to this review. The term “body modifications” is broad and can include everything listed in Table 1, as well as extreme dieting, bodybuilding, cosmetic procedures (e.g., lip fillers and muscle relaxing injections) and surgeries (e.g., breast augmentation and fat removal or displacement), and even hair dying. However, for this scoping review, we opted to include only invasive, voluntary body modification processes done outside of medical contexts. To focus on discipline-specific approaches that might explain the persistence of stigmatisation in the literature, further inclusion criteria were: (1) research published in peer-reviewed psychology journals or multidisciplinary journals; (2) where papers are published in multidisciplinary journals, at least one of the authors is a psychologist or psychiatrist (deduced by affiliation); (3) papers published in English; and (4) research is empirical or theoretical (primary source, not a review). Research was excluded if these criteria were not met. Qualitative and quantitative research was eligible, and all years of publication were included in the search.

We searched the following databases on 13 April 2020 and updated on 17 May 2021: Embase Ovid, Web of Science, EBSCOhost and PubMed. The search specified tattoo* OR piercing OR scarification in the title AND feeling OR motivation OR attitude OR perception in the abstract. Further searches across these databases were conducted for “extreme modification” in the title OR in the abstract to try and identify any relevant journal articles regarding less popular forms of body modification. Search results were filtered to journal articles only and limited to relevant academic disciplines (for example, psychology, psychiatry, social sciences). We conducted a final search of Google Scholar on 7 April 2022 for any new relevant publications. Finally, ad hoc articles were found in the course of research through miscellaneous means, such as through being referenced in another article.

Through title screening, we identified 297 articles from the four databases and 21 from other sources (Fig. 1). We removed 54 duplicate articles identified in multiple databases. We used the inclusion/exclusion criteria for final sample determination. Articles for which study objectives or author affiliations were unclear were examined closely by all five co-authors and discussed further. For instance, several articles on body modification appear in the interdisciplinary journal Deviant Behaviour by authors from various disciplines, so only those by authors with express affiliations to psychology or psychiatry were retained. Other close-but-no cases were an article in the journal Art Therapy by an art therapist and several articles in nursing journals by nursing faculty exploring tattooing motivations and identity-formation. Art therapists are sometimes psychologically trained, so we examined the credentials of the author, and, since the degrees were in counselling and therapy but not psychology or psychiatry explicitly, the article was excluded. The nursing articles are like the psychology/psychiatry journal articles thematically in their focus on motivations and stigma, but they do not meet inclusion criteria for author affiliations. This process led to the exclusion of 171 articles, leaving 94 eligible for analysis.

Deviance as an historical artefact: a scoping review of psychological studies of body modification (1)

PRISMA flow diagram of the scoping review process.

Our team of five co-authors met weekly over the course of a year to read and discuss coding of articles. First, we divided the articles up among our team, read 5–10 each, and identified salient themes. We then met to discuss the themes we had identified and repeated this process multiple times. Through this iterative process, we determined that the best way to assess the corpus of psychological studies of body modifications would be to categorise them based on their stated or implied study objectives. Therefore, we reshuffled the articles and divided them evenly among the five team members, and each team member categorised the articles based on the specific or apparent objectives of each article’s authors. We then met as a team and discussed the coding of each article; if the team was not in agreement, we discussed until we reached consensus on the final set of categories and subcategories and the categories to which each article should be assigned. Ad hoc articles discovered during the process were reviewed together and assigned categories by the entire team.

In addition to themes, we coded articles based the demographic focus of the study (e.g., college students, women, prisoners, etc.), what country the study took place in and whether they were in-person or online studies, the methods used, and gaps or critiques of the literature identified by the authors. After coding the articles, we reread the articles within each section in order of publication date to see what theory authors were drawing on and, a task which elicited some additional articles and shed light on temporal changes in Psychology as a discipline.

Results

We identified 69 articles about tattooing only, 18 about tattooing and piercing, 6 about piercing only, and one article about multiple forms of body modifications, including scarification, tattooing, piercing, and other forms.

Table 2 outlines the categories that most appropriately characterise the objectives of each article. The most common sample among psychology articles examining body modifications was a general Euro-American population (n = 33), followed by college students (n = 22), women only (n = 9, including one that sampled women only but asked about men), prisoners or convicts (n = 7), patients (n = 6), youth (n = 4), specific job roles (n = 4), general tattooed population (n = 3), case studies (n = 2), men only (n = 2, one sampled men but asked about women only), and multiple populations (n = 2).

The most common objective among psychology articles about body modifications (n = 36 or 38% of total sample) was to test for underlying dysfunction or tendencies toward deviant behaviour associated with body modifications. Among those, 25% examined correlations between past behaviours or experiences and body modifications, whereas 41% examined current behaviours or sought to predict future behaviour.

The second most common objective was an effort to assess how tattooed (not body modifications in general) people are perceived, characterised, and treated by others (n = 31 or 32% of total sample). Within this category, we identified six sub-objectives, including perceptions for employment or when at work (32% of category), if tattooing impacts trustworthiness (12% of category), if tattooed people are worth helping (9% of category), and if tattooing influences perceived attractiveness (6% of category).

The third most common objective was a general exploration of why people engage in body modifications (n = 18 or 19% of total sample). Among those, five articles (27% of category) explored the possibility that tattooed people are fundamentally different from non-tattooed people.

The fourth most common objective of psychological studies of body modifications was to explore if tattoos impact health (n = 15 or 16% of total sample). Two articles (13% of category) concluded that tattoos help health, whereas authors of nine articles (67% of category) investigated whether tattoos indicate poor health.

The least common objective among psychological studies of body modifications was to explore them as aspects of identity (n = 9 or 9% of total sample). We identified one subcategory for those focused on subjective feelings of attractiveness vis-à-vis their body modifications with two articles in this subcategory (22% of category).

Discussion

In this article, we provide an overview of peer-reviewed, primary source articles on voluntary, invasive body modifications published in psychology journals or journal articles that featured one or more authors whose affiliation was a Psychology or Psychiatry programme. Our search was intended to be a comprehensive assessment of sources available through online databases. The identified studies range widely in terms of study characteristics, methodologies, and locations. A notable finding was that there were few psychological/psychiatric studies of body modifications beyond those related to tattooing. Most of the body modifications outlined in Table 1 are rare, so, if the objective of the studies was to identify aberration, it might seem intuitive to study the rates and motivations for engaging in rare body modifications. However, our main finding is that psychological/psychiatric studies of tattooing have been rooted in traditional abnormal psychology and have tended to reify stigma through their research design. This is true even when the author objectives are to demonstrate that modified people are not deviant or stigmatised. There were very few psychological studies of body modifications outside of clinical or penal settings until the twenty-first century, suggesting changing perspectives in the fields of psychology and psychiatry.

Body modifications and deviance

Most psychological studies of body modifications seem to derive from the field of abnormal and clinical psychology. The earliest publications in our sample (Duncan, 1989; Edgerton and Dingman, 1963; Ferguson-Rayport et al., 1955; Taylor, 1974) are studies of prisoners and psychiatric patients that provide clues as to the shift from popular practice to stigma. Ferguson-Rayport et al. (1955) review numerous studies indicating, for instance, that tattooed people were more likely to be denied military enlistment or that tattooing was linked with hom*osexual behaviour in correctional institutions and reform schools. Furthermore, the authors hold “that the tattoo expresses masoch*stic-exhibitionistic drives and directly illustrates and encourages hom*osexual activity…tattoos are often compensatory in individuals poorly adjusted, especially in the sexual sphere” (Ferguson-Rayport et al., 1955, p. 116). Such studies appear to reconstitute or extend earlier criminological efforts to taxonomically categorise potential for deviance. Edgerton and Dingman (1963), by contrast, conducted a qualitative exploration of tattooing among mental hospital patients to determine if, as suggested by anthropological studies of non-Euro-american tattoo practices, marking oneself permanently is an important aspect of identity-formation.

Though several standardised methods for assessing personality were developed in the first half of the twentieth century (Butcher, 2009), they do not appear to have been used in body modification research until the 1970s, when research sought to determine if tattooed prisoners and psychiatric patients were psychologically different than non-tattooed counterparts. For example, Taylor (1974) indicates significant differences between tattooed and non-tattooed individuals but does not provide any statistics to support this. By contrast, Duncan (1989) and other forensic studies (e.g., Birmingham et al., 1999) find negative or unhealthy behaviour associated variously with both modified and non-modified inmates.

Within the clinical psychology literature, penal or forensic studies seem to highlight body modifications as potential indicators of deviance or mental disorder, whereas studies relating to outpatient disorders (e.g., Bui et al., 2013; Caplan et al., 1996; Claes et al., 2005) suggest that body modifications may be sublimations of tendencies toward self-harm or other negative behaviour. Cardasis et al. (2008) notes a justification for this approach, pointing out that a primary feature of anti-social personality disorder is a need to seek immediate gratification and external stimulation to alleviate anxiety or discomfort. Buss and Hodges (2017) suggest the search for associations between body modifications and deviance is rooted in religious proscriptions against marking one’s body (see Scheinfeld, 2007 for examples) that were employed in the colonial era of empire-building to distinguish the “civilised” from “savage”. Multiple studies (e.g., Aizenman and Jensen, 2007; Ceylan et al., 2019; Stirn and Hinz, 2008; Stirn et al., 2011; Swami et al., 2015; Vizgaitis and Lenzenweger, 2019) conflate body modifications and non-suicidal self-injury or trauma and suggest that body modifications may be indicators of other anti-social tendencies.

Others (e.g., Drews et al., 2000) simply choose to emphasise terms like “risky” behaviour over analogous but differently valanced terms like “adventurous” or to emphasise the potential risks of tattooing (e.g., Koch, Roberts, Cannon, et al., 2005). Though poor-quality tattooing can certainly be dangerous (Kluger, 2015; Kluger and Koljonen, 2012), the chances of encountering tattoo-related medical complications in the current era is low, and there is some evidence to suggest that past fears over blood-borne pathogen transmission have been greater than the actual incidence of such tattoo-related medical complications (Jelinski, 2018; Lynn et al., 2019).

Body modifications and social interactions

All other psychological studies seem to wrestle with the changing status of body modifications as an emerging or “new” normal. We identified what could be called a “general” category of social psychology studies of body modifications, seeking explanations for how modified people are perceived and how people with modifications are treated (e.g., Drews et al., 2000; Galbarczyk et al., 2020; Galbarczyk and Ziomkiewicz, 2017; Hawkes et al., 2004; Martino, 2008; Martino and Lester, 2011; Miłkowska et al., 2018; Resenhoeft et al., 2008; Wohlrab et al., 2009a, 2009b). An a priori assumption undergirding these studies is that body modifications have been historically stigmatised, and stigma may persist in interpersonal interactions.

A paradigm shift in body modification research seems to have occurred from the 1970s through the 2000s, with psychology the last to come around. This shift makes the fields of psychology and psychiatry appear to be out of step, but it is worth remembering that clinical psychology is historically grounded in the deficit-oriented biomedical model, which is focused on healing illness and diagnosing disfunction (Sheridan and Radmacher, 1992). Whereas body modifications as normal behaviours have long been subjects of study for allied social sciences like anthropology and sociology, social psychology was still developing as a discipline in the 1950s and 1960s (Stangor, 2014); social psychology research on body modifications seems to have only gotten underway beginning in the twenty-first century.

Three areas within social psychology seem particularly focused on body modifications: industrial, health, and evolutionary psychology. Industrial or occupational psychology is a subfield of social psychology concerned with human relations in work-related settings. A common theme in the concern over body modifications is how visible modifications will influence employability (e.g., Burgess and Clark, 2010; Dillingh et al., 2020; Flanagan and Lewis, 2019; Hauke-Forman et al., 2021; Tews et al., 2020; Thielgen et al., 2020; Timming et al., 2017; Timming, 2015; 2017; Wiseman, 2010b). Some studies relate to particular circ*mstances wherein bias toward body modifications could undermine interactions beyond employment status, such as in the courtroom (e.g., Funk and Todorov, 2013) or classroom (Wiseman, 2010b) or based on the specific imagery of a person’s tattoos (e.g., Timming and Perrett, 2016).

Among health psychologists, there is concern that the association of body modifications with abnormality might lead to people in marginal groups being in “double jeopardy” (e.g., Zestcott et al., 2017; Zestcott and Stone, 2019; Zestcott et al., 2018). Another line of research taking the opposite tack comes out of evolutionary psychology, arguably a subfield of social psychology (Kruglanski and Wolfgang, 2012). The evolutionary perspective suggests that well-healed modifications may function as external indicators of good underlying health. This hypothesis is tested by exploring how modifications are perceived by observers in terms of attractiveness and health as adaptive indicators of partner suitability (e.g., Galbarczyk et al., 2020; Galbarczyk and Ziomkiewicz, 2017; Miłkowska et al., 2018; Wohlrab et al., 2009a, 2009b).

Body modifications and health

Most studies of body modifications and health do not focus on the double jeopardy of marginalised groups or evolutionary signalling theory. Most are grounded in abnormal psychology studies of the 1980s and 1990s and therefore collect data on mental/physical health and substance use even when their study objective is ostensibly about body modifications and other topics (e.g., Dillingh et al., 2020). Some reframe the focus on abnormality and instead use body modifications as indications of “impulsivity” or tendencies toward “sensation-seeking”, which are themselves considered risk factors for some abnormalities (e.g., Kertzman et al., 2013; Mortensen et al., 2019). One study uncritically claims that tattoos and premarital sex are “categorically deviant in a traditional sense but are typical among college students” as justification for a correlational study (Koch et al., 2005, p. 887). Another study by the same group of authors (Koch et al., 2005) explores beliefs about the health and social dangers of tattoos, as though tattoos are more dangerous than current evidence suggests (cf. Jelinski, 2018; Lynn and Medeiros, 2017). Some studies note that, not only are body modifications different in how they are adopted and used, but each type of body modification also has variation. Tattooing varies by design, extent, body location and other factors that are “read” by interlocutors and observers in the explicit and implicit communication of social interactions (Geller et al., 2020). Furthermore, as body modifications become more popular in developing countries due to media exposure, psychological studies conducted by researchers in those countries replicate the type of mental health studies conducted previously in Europe and the United States (e.g., Geller et al., 2020; Kertzman et al., 2019a, 2019b; Kertzman et al., 2013; Pajor et al., 2015).

By contrast, other psychological studies acknowledge that, among body modifications, piercing and tattooing is “becoming mainstream” (Hill et al., 2016, p. 246) and explore body modifications from developmental psychology perspectives. Some such studies explore body modifications as means of improving self-esteem or one’s own body image (e.g., Hill et al., 2016; Kertzman et al., 2019a), as a form of healing from trauma (e.g., Maxwell et al., 2020), or as an option for healthy lifestyles (Huxley and Grogan, 2005). One unique study (Thompson, 2015) explored associations between tattooing and “generativity”, a concept associated with prosocial behaviour.

Tattooing and identity

Beyond the importance of deviance, health, and the social roles of tattoos, researchers identified the role and materiality of body modifications in identity and personal aesthetics. We note two general trends concerning the role of body modifications specifically in emerging identities and perceptions of attractiveness. We distinguished studies of identity as those in which researchers largely ask questions that explore how those with body modifications view themselves. Within these articles, there are two further distinctions of identity, where researchers identify the underlying meanings attributed to body modifications (e.g., Mun et al., 2012; Tiggemann and Golder, 2006; Tiggemann and Hopkins, 2011) and modifications as identity signalling to social others (e.g., Bergh et al., 2017; Dillingh et al., 2020; Molloy and Wagstaff, 2021; Mun et al., 2012; Skegg et al., 2007).

Neither identity nor the meanings people attribute to their tattoos are fixed in time (Howson, 2013), and researchers try to convey this complexity by collecting diachronic information pertaining to the meaning of tattoos (the narrative story behind the tattoo), though the recurrent theme within these studies concerns change to meanings (e.g., Mun et al., 2012; Tiggemann and Golder, 2006; Tiggemann and Hopkins, 2011). For example, Mun et al. (2012) note that some tattoo meanings shift over time, reflecting life transitions, such as tattoos that commemorate past relationships or affiliations (e.g., gang imagery) (e.g., Mun et al., 2012). The through line of these identity-oriented articles largely point to individuals using tattoos as markers of individuality that reflect multi-faceted, densely layered meanings.

Tattoos can also be the material manifestations of certain types of communal (e.g., Edgerton and Dingman, 1963), ethnic (e.g., Skegg et al., 2007), or gender identities (e.g., Galbarczyk and Ziomkiewicz, 2017). Within this scoping review, several of the developmental psychology articles focus largely on tattoos as materialising and signalling identity (e.g., Dillingh et al., 2020; Mun et al., 2012; Skegg et al., 2007). Although many individuals use tattoos to signal identity through self-presentation (e.g., Molloy and Wagstaff, 2021; Mun et al., 2012; Tiggemann and Golder, 2006; Tiggemann and Hopkins, 2011), this requires understanding the meaning of a tattoo’s visibility (can it be seen by casual observer or only in intimate circ*mstances?) (Dillingh et al., 2020). People present themselves in myriad situations and environments in their everyday lives, requiring different “selves” to be presented accordingly; tattoos signalling identity can therefore take many forms, and psychologists suggest that people make these decisions based on life experiences, social settings, and other reasons in order to embody the identities they want to present (e.g., Dillingh et al., 2020; Mun et al., 2012; Tiggemann and Golder, 2006; Tiggemann and Hopkins, 2011).

Limitations

Our analysis unfortunately reinforces the “siloing” of academic disciplines for a subject that is in fact very interdisciplinary in nature and has been studied from numerous vantages we did not address. However, the historical trend we have noted was not apparent until we conducted this analysis, and it is important to distinguish the contributions various disciplines can make to body modification research and what strengths and weaknesses may be inherent to respective disciplinary approaches. This analysis may also suggest that psychologists and psychiatrists are alone in drawing parallels between body modification and risk behaviour or stigma, but this is also far from true. Nevertheless, forensic research conducted by psychologists, psychiatrists, and criminologists prevails among early body modification studies. Future research should include similar treatments for other relevant disciplines (e.g., anthropology, sociology, biology, criminology, nursing, dermatology, etc.).

Conclusions

The psychological studies examined in our review span the period 1955–2021. Early studies imply moral parallelisms by comparing body modification tendencies to religiosity, sexual activity, sexuality, alcohol or drug use, etc. This approach seems to derive from the legacy of nineteenth century criminology, which in turn appears based on a previous stigmatisation of irreversible body alterations among European cultures (Caplan, 1997). Lane (2014) makes a similar observation, suggesting that nineteenth century criminologists thought of criminals as atavistic and tattoos as indications of their reversion to primitiveness. This approach is continued in contemporary research when studying body modification in clinical populations, as well as among adolescents, seeking explanations for past behaviours and for “tells” of future tendencies (Lane, 2014).

In conclusion, we found no legitimate motivation for the inherent stigma towards individuals who voluntarily modify their bodies. Instead, this is an historically particular legacy of the social sciences and their various developments. Continued focus on deviance or risk regarding body modifications directly (i.e., not including intervening assessments of personality traits) seems a desperate assertion of an antiquated or atypical moral stance that now rings as somewhat absurd. Future research should continue to integrate perspectives from allied disciplines to gain a more accurate and nuanced view of the psychology of body modifications. The psychobiosocial approach taken in several twenty-first century health psychology studies—e.g., “double jeopardy” among marginalised populations or how tattoos are used by some people to help heal from past traumas—are promising research directions. The psychological study of modified people has primarily focused on tattooing, and future research should also acknowledge the variation of invasive voluntary modifications as they become more popular and available. Furthermore, technological advances are making tattoos less permanent while opening biomedicine to other forms of tattooing, which promise further shifts in how we study the psychology of body modifications. Emerging research using new methods and technology and that acknowledges how past research design reify the stigma they purport to study promises suggests a new paradigm of body modification investigations on the horizon.

Data availability

Data sharing is not applicable to this research as no data were generated or analysed.

References

  • Aizenman M, Jensen MAC (2007) Speaking through the body: the incidence of self‐injury, piercing, and tattooing among college students. J College Counsel 10(1):27–43

    Article Google Scholar

  • Anderson M, Sansone RA (2003) Tattooing as a means of acute affect regulation. Clin Psychol & Psychother 10(5):316–318

    Article Google Scholar

  • Angel G (2017) Recovering the nineteenth-century European tattoo. In: Ancient ink: the Archaeology of tattooing. (pp. 107–129). Seattle: University of Washington Press.

  • Antoszewski B, Sitek A, Fijałkowska M, Kasielska A, Kruk-Jeromin J (2010) Tattooing and body piercing—what motivates you to do it. Int J Soc Psychiatry 56(5):471. https://doi.org/10.1177/0020764009106253

    Article Google Scholar

  • Atkinson M (2004) Tattooing and civilizing processes: body modification as self-control. Can Rev Sociol 41(2):125. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1755-618X.2004.tb02173.x

    Article Google Scholar

  • Ball J, Elsner RJ (2017) Changes in aesthetic appeal of tattoos are influenced by the attractiveness of female models. Psychology 7(4):247–252

    Google Scholar

  • Bergh L, Jordaan J, Lombard E, Naude L, van Zyl J (2017) Social media, permanence, and tattooed students: the case for personal, personal branding. Crit Arts 31(4):1–17

    Article Google Scholar

  • Birmingham L, Mason D, Grubin D (1999) The psychiatric implications of visible tattoos in an adult male prison population. J Forens Psychiatry 10(3):687–695

    Article Google Scholar

  • Bradley J (2000) Body commodification? Class and tattoos in Victorian Britain. In: Caplan J (ed.), Written on the body: the tattoo in European and American History . Princeton University Press. p. 136

  • Broussard KA, Harton HC (2018) Tattoo or taboo? Tattoo stigma and negative attitudes toward tattooed individuals. J Soc Psychol 158(5):521–540. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.2017.1373622

    Article Google Scholar

  • Bui E, Rodgers R, Simon NM, Jehel L, Metcalf CA, Birmes P, Schmitt L (2013) Body piercings and posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms in young adults. Stress Health 29(1):70–74

    Article Google Scholar

  • Burgess M, Clark L (2010) Do the “savage origins” of tattoos cast a prejudicial shadow on contemporary tattooed individuals? J Appl Soc Psychol 40(3):746–764

    Article Google Scholar

  • Burns DL (2019) Social media and tattoo culture in consideration of gender. In: Bodinger-deUriante C (ed.). Interfacing ourselves: living in the digital age. Routledge. pp. 28–42

  • Buss L, Hodges K (2017) Marked: tattoo as an expression of psyche. Psychol Perspect 60(1):4–38

    Article Google Scholar

  • Butcher JN (2009) Clinical personality assessment: History, evolution, contemporary models, and practical applications. Oxford handbook of personality assessment. Oxford University Press. pp. 5–21

  • Caplan J (1997) ‘Speaking scars’: the tattoo in popular practice and medico-legal debate in nineteenth-century Europe. In: History Workshop Journal, No. 44, Autumn, 1997, p. 129

  • Caplan J (2000a) ‘National Tattooing’: traditions of tattooing in nineteenth-century Europe. In: Caplan J (ed.), Written on the Body: The Tattoo in European and American History. Princeton University Press. pp. 156–173

  • Caplan J (ed.) (2000b). Written on the body: the tattoo in European and American history. Princeton University Press

  • Caplan R, Komaromi J, Rhodes M (1996) Obsessive–compulsive disorder, tattooing and bizarre sexual practices. Br J Psychiatry 168(3):379–380

    Article CAS Google Scholar

  • Cardasis W, Huth‐Bocks A, Silk KR (2008) Tattoos and antisocial personality disorder. Pers Mental Health 2(3):171–182

    Article Google Scholar

  • Carroll L, Anderson R (2002) Body piercing, tattooing, self-esteem, and body investment in adolescent girls. Adolescence 37(147):627

    Google Scholar

  • Ceylan MF, Hesapcioglu ST, Kasak M, Yavas CP (2019) High prevalence of nonsuicidal self-injury, tattoos, and psychiatric comorbidity among male adolescent prisoners and their sociodemographic characteristics. Asian J Psychiatry 43:45–49

    Article Google Scholar

  • Claes L, Vandereycken W, Vertommen H (2005) Self‐care versus self‐harm: Piercing, tattooing, and self‐injuring in eating disorders. Eur Eat Disorder Rev 13(1):11–18

    Article Google Scholar

  • DeMello M (2000) Bodies of inscription: A cultural history of the modern tattoo community. Duke University Press. http://scout.lib.ua.edu.libdata.lib.ua.edu/?itemid=library/m/voyager1040732

  • Department SR (2021, 1 June). Body modification in the United States-Statistics & Facts. Statista. Retrieved 20 May from https://www.statista.com/topics/5135/body-modification-in-the-us/#topicHeader__wrapper

  • Deter-Wolf A, Robitaille B, Krutak L, Galliot S (2016) The world’s oldest tattoos. J Archaeol Sci Rep 5:19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jasrep.2015.11.007

    Article Google Scholar

  • Dhossche D, Snell KS, Larder S (2000) A case-control study of tattoos in young suicide victims as a possible marker of risk. J Affect Disord 59(2):165–168

    Article CAS Google Scholar

  • Dillingh R, Kooreman P, Potters J (2020) Tattoos, lifestyle, and the labor market. Labour 34(2):191–214

    Article Google Scholar

  • Douglas B (2005) Cureous figures: European voyagers and tatau/tattoo in Polynesia, 1595-1800. In: Thomas N, Cole A, Douglas B (eds.), Tattoo: Bodies, art and exchange in the Pacific and the West. Duke University Press. pp. 33–52

  • Drews DR, Allison CK, Probst JR (2000) Behavioral and self-concept differences in tattooed and nontattooed college students. Psychol Rep 86(2):475–481

    Article CAS Google Scholar

  • Duncan DF (1989) MMPI scores of tattooed and untattooed prisoners. Psychol Rep 65(2):685–686

    Article CAS Google Scholar

  • Edgerton RB, Dingman HF (1963) Tattooing and identity. Int J Soc Psychiatry 9(2):143. https://doi.org/10.1177/002076406300900211

    Article Google Scholar

  • Ekinci O, Topcuoglu V, Sabuncuoglu O, Berkem M, Akin E, Gumustas FO (2012) The association of tattooing/body piercing and psychopathology in adolescents: a community based study from Istanbul. Commun Ment Health J 48(6):798–803

    Article Google Scholar

  • Eubanks V (1996) Zones of Dither: writing the postmodern body. Body Soc 2(3):73. https://doi.org/10.1177/1357034×96002003004

    Article Google Scholar

  • Ferguson-Rayport SM, Griffith RM, Straus EW (1955) The psychiatric significance of tattoos. Psychiatric Q 29(1):112–131. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01567443

    Article CAS Google Scholar

  • Flanagan JL, Lewis VJ (2019) Marked inside and out: an exploration of perceived stigma of the tattooed in the workplace. Equal, Divers Inclus Int J, 38(1):87-106. https://doi.org/10.1108/EDI-06-2018-0101

  • Forbes GB (2001) College students with tattoos and piercings: Motives, family experiences, personality factors, and perception by others. Psychol Rep 89(3):774. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.2001.89.3.774

    Article CAS Google Scholar

  • Funk F, Todorov A (2013) Criminal stereotypes in the courtroom: Facial tattoos affect guilt and punishment differently. Psychol Public Policy Law 19(4):466

    Article Google Scholar

  • Galbarczyk A, Mijas M, Marcinkowska UM, Koziara K, Apanasewicz A, Ziomkiewicz A (2020) Association between sexual orientations of individuals and perceptions of tattooed men. Psychol Sex 11(3):150–160

    Google Scholar

  • Galbarczyk A, Ziomkiewicz A (2017) Tattooed men: healthy bad boys and good-looking competitors. Pers Individ Differs 106:122–125

    Article Google Scholar

  • Geller S, Magen E, Levy S, Handelzalts J (2020) Inkskinned: gender and personality aspects affecting heavy tattooing—a moderation model. J Adult Dev 27(3):192–200

    Article Google Scholar

  • Govenar A (2000) The changing image of tattooing in American culture, 1846-1966. In: Caplan J (ed.), Written on the body: the tattoo in European and American history. Princeton University Press. p. 212

  • Guéguen N (2012a) Tattoos, piercings, and alcohol consumption. Alcoholism: Clin Exp Res 36(7):1253–1256

    Article Google Scholar

  • Guéguen N (2012b) Tattoos, piercings, and sexual activity. Soc Behav Pers: Int J 40(9):1543–1547

    Article Google Scholar

  • Guéguen N (2013) Effects of a tattoo on men’s behavior and attitudes towards women: an experimental field study. Arch Sex Behav 42(8):1517–1524

    Article Google Scholar

  • Hauke-Forman N, Methner N, Bruckmüller S (2021) Assertive, but less competent and trustworthy? perception of police officers with tattoos and piercings. J Police Criml Psychol 36(3):523–536

    Article Google Scholar

  • Hawkes D, Senn CY, Thorn C (2004) Factors that influence attitudes toward women with tattoos. Sex Roles 50(9):593–604

    Article Google Scholar

  • Hill BM, Ogletree S, McCrary K (2016) Body modifications in college students: considering gender, self-esteem, body appreciation, and reasons for tattoos. Coll Stud J 50(2):246–252

    Google Scholar

  • Howson A (2013) The body in society: an introduction. John Wiley & Sons

  • Huxley C, Grogan S (2005) Tattooing, piercing, healthy behaviours and health value. J Health Psychol 10(6):831. https://doi.org/10.1177/1359105305057317

    Article Google Scholar

  • Jaworska K, Fijałkowska M, Antoszewski B (2018) Tattoos yesterday and today in the Polish population in the decade 2004–2014. Health Psychology Report 6(4):321–329

    Article Google Scholar

  • Jelinski J (2018) Bad bastards? tattooing, health, and regulation in twentieth-century vancouve. Urban Hist Rev/Revue d’histoire urbaine 47(1/2):103–114. https://www.jstor.org/stable/26841717

    Google Scholar

  • Jennings WG, Fox BH, Farrington DP (2014) Inked into crime? An examination of the causal relationship between tattoos and life-course offending among males from the Cambridge Study in Delinquent Development. J Crim Just 42(1):77–84

    Article Google Scholar

  • Kalanj-Mizzi SA, Snell TL, Simmonds JG (2019) Motivations for multiple tattoo acquisition: an interpretative phenomenological analysis. Adv Men Health 17(2):196–213

    Article Google Scholar

  • Kertzman S, Kagan A, Hegedish O, Lapidus R, Weizman A (2019a) Do young women with tattoos have lower self-esteem and body image than their peers without tattoos? A non-verbal repertory grid technique approach. PLoS ONE 14(1):e0206411

    Article CAS Google Scholar

  • Kertzman S, Kagan A, Hegedish O, Lapidus R, Weizman A (2019b) The role of inhibition capacities in the Iowa gambling test performance in young tattooed women. BMC Psychol 7(1):1–9

    Article Google Scholar

  • Kertzman S, Kagan A, Vainder M, Lapidus R, Weizman A (2013) Interactions between risky decisions, impulsiveness and smoking in young tattooed women. BMC Psychiatry 13(1):1–8

    Article Google Scholar

  • Kluger N (2015) Epidemiology of tattoos in industrialized countries. Curr Prob Dermatol 48:6. https://doi.org/10.1159/000369175

    Article Google Scholar

  • Kluger N, Koljonen V (2012) Tattoos, inks, and cancer. Lancet Oncol 13(4):e161. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1470-2045(11)70340-0

    Article Google Scholar

  • Koch JR, Roberts AE, Armstrong ML, Owen DC (2005) College students, tattoos, and sexual activity. Psychol Rep 97(3):887–890

    Article Google Scholar

  • Koch JR, Roberts AE, Cannon JH, Armstrong ML, Owen DC (2005) College students, tattooing, and the health belief model: extending social psychological perspectives on youth culture and deviance. Sociol Spectr 25(1):79–102

    Article Google Scholar

  • Kruglanski AW, Wolfgang S (2012). The making of social psychology. In: Handbook of the history of social psychology. Psychology Press. pp. 3–17

  • Lane DC (2014) Tat’s all folks: an analysis of tattoo literature. Sociol Compass 8(4):398–410

    Article Google Scholar

  • Lingel J, Boyd D (2013) “Keep it secret, keep it safe”: information poverty, information norms, and stigma. J Am Soc Inform Sci Technol 64(5):981–991. https://doi.org/10.1002/asi.22800

    Article Google Scholar

  • Lodder M (2013) Neo-Victorian tattooing. Victoriana: a miscellany. Guildhall Art Gallery, London, p 105–112

    Google Scholar

  • Lynn CD, Medeiros CA (2017) Tattooing commitment, quality, and football in Southeastern North America. In: Lynn CD, Glaze AL, Evans WA, Reed LK (eds.). Evolution education in the American South. Palgrave Macmillan. p. 263

  • Lynn CD, Puckett T, Guitar A, Roy N (2019) Shirts or skins?: Tattoos as costly honest signals of fitness and affiliation among US intercollegiate athletes and other undergraduates. Evol Psychol Sci 5:151–165. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40806-018-0174-4

    Article Google Scholar

  • Manuel L, Retzlaff PD (2002) Psychopathology and tattooing among prisoners. Int J Offender Ther Comparat Criminol 46(5):522–531

    Article Google Scholar

  • Martin A (1997) On teenagers and tattoos. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 36(6):860–861. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199706000-00025

  • Martino S (2008) Perceptions of a photograph of a woman with visible piercings. Psychol Rep 103(1):134–138

    Article Google Scholar

  • Martino S, Lester D (2011) Perceptions of visible piercings: a pilot study. Psychol Rep 109(3):755–758

    Article Google Scholar

  • Maxwell D, Thomas J, Thomas SA (2020) Cathartic ink: a qualitative examination of tattoo motivations for survivors of sexual trauma. Deviant Behav 41(3):348–365

    Article Google Scholar

  • McCabe M (1997) New York City tattoo: The oral history of an urban art. Hardy Marks Publications

  • Miłkowska K, Ziomkiewicz A, Galbarczyk A (2018) Tattooed man: could menstrual cycle phase and contraceptive use change female preferences towards bad boys. Pers Individ Differ 121(Supplement C):41. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.09.017

    Article Google Scholar

  • Molloy K, Wagstaff D (2021) Effects of gender, self-rated attractiveness, and mate value on perceptions tattoos. Pers Individ Differ 168:110382

    Article Google Scholar

  • Mortensen K, French MT, Timming AR (2019) Are tattoos associated with negative health‐related outcomes and risky behaviors? Int J Dermatol 58(7):816–824

    Article Google Scholar

  • Mun JM, Janigo KA, Johnson KK (2012) Tattoo and the self. Cloth Text Res J 30(2):134–148

    Article Google Scholar

  • Naudé L, Jordaan J, Bergh L (2019) “My body is my journal, and my tattoos are my story”: South African psychology students’ reflections on tattoo practices. Curr Psychol 38(1):177–186

    Article Google Scholar

  • Oettermann S (2000) On display: tattooed entertainers in America and Germany. In: Caplan J (ed.). Written on the body: the tattoo in European and American history. Princeton University Press. pp. 193

  • Pajor AJ, Broniarczyk-Dyła G, Świtalska J (2015) Satisfaction with life, self-esteem and evaluation of mental health in people with tattoos or piercings. Psychiatria Polska 49(3):559–573

    Article Google Scholar

  • Phillips M (2022) Seeing tattoos makes me feel physically sick: Ubiquity of body art is born out of an existential crisis of humanity in the post-religious world. The Times. https://www.thetimes.co.uk/article/seeing-tattoos-makes-me-feel-physically-sick-6gxpjv85t

  • Pirrone C, Castellano S, Platania GA, Ruggieri S, Caponnetto P (2020) Comparing the emerging psychological meaning of tattoos in drug-addicted and not drug-addicted adults: a look inside health risks. Health Psychol Res 8(2):9268. https://doi.org/10.4081/hpr.2020.9268

  • Pitts-Taylor V (2003) In the flesh: The cultural politics of body modification. Palgrave Macmillan. http://scout.lib.ua.edu/?itemid=library/m/voyager1568152

  • Resenhoeft A, Villa J, Wiseman D (2008) Tattoos can harm perceptions: a study and suggestions. J Am Coll Health 56(5):593. https://doi.org/10.3200/jach.56.5.593-596

    Article Google Scholar

  • Roberti JW, Storch EA (2005) Psychosocial adjustment of college students with tattoos and piercings. J College Counsel 8(1):14–19. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2161-1882.2005.tb00068.x

    Article Google Scholar

  • Roberts TA, Auinger P, Ryan SA (2004) Body piercing and high-risk behavior in adolescents. J Adolesc Health 34(3):224–229. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2003.06.005

    Article Google Scholar

  • Rubin A (1988) The tattoo renaissance. In: Rubin A (ed.). Marks of civilization: artistic transformations of the human body. University of California Museum of Cultural History. p. 233

  • Ruffle BJ, Wilson AE (2019) Tat will tell: tattoos and time preferences. J Econ Behav Organ 166:566–585. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2019.08.001

    Article Google Scholar

  • Sagoe D, Pallesen S, Andreassen CS (2017) Prevalence and correlates of tattooing in Norway: a large-scale cross-sectional study. Scand J Psychol 58(6):562–570. https://doi.org/10.1111/sjop.12399

    Article Google Scholar

  • Scheinfeld N (2007) Tattoos and religion. Clinics in dermatology 25(4):362. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clindermatol.2007.05.009

    Article Google Scholar

  • Schlösser A, Giacomozzi AI, Camargo BV, Silva EZPD, Xavier M (2020) Mujeres con Tatuajes y sin Tatuajes: Motivaciones, Prácticas Sociales y Comportamiento de Riesgo. Psico-USF 25(1):51–62

    Article Google Scholar

  • Seiter JS, Hatch S (2005) Effect of tattoos on perceptions of credibility and attractiveness. Psychol Rep 96(3_suppl):1113–1120

    Article Google Scholar

  • Sheridan CL, Radmacher SA (1992) Health psychology: Challenging the biomedical model. John Wiley & Sons

  • Skegg K, Nada-Raja S, Paul C, Skegg DC (2007) Body piercing, personality, and sexual behavior. Arch Sex Behav 36(1):47–54

    Article Google Scholar

  • Skoda K, Oswald F, Brown K, Hesse C, Pedersen CL (2020) Showing skin: Tattoo visibility status, egalitarianism, and personality are predictors of sexual openness among women. Sex Cult 24(6):1935–1956

    Article Google Scholar

  • Solís-Bravo MA, Flores-Rodríguez Y, Tapia-Guillen LG, Gatica-Hernández A, Guzmán-Reséndiz M, Salinas-Torres LA, Vargas-Rizo TL, Albores-Gallo L (2019) Are tattoos an indicator of severity of non-suicidal self-injury behavior in adolescents? Psychiatry Investig 16(7):504

    Article Google Scholar

  • Stangor C (2014) Defining social psychology: history and principles. Principles of social psychology-1st International edn. BCcampus Open Education

  • Stirn A, Hinz A (2008) Tattoos, body piercings, and self-injury: Is there a connection? Investigations on a core group of participants practicing body modification. Psychother Res 18(3):326–333

    Article Google Scholar

  • Stirn A, Hinz A, Brahler E (2006) Prevalence of tattooing and body piercing in Germany and perception of health, mental disorders, and sensation seeking among tattooed and body-pierced individuals. J Psychosomat Res 60(5):531. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychores.2005.09.002

    Article Google Scholar

  • Stirn A, Oddo S, Peregrinova L, Philipp S, Hinz A (2011) Motivations for body piercings and tattoos —the role of sexual abuse and the frequency of body modifications. Psychiatry Res 190(2):359–363. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2011.06.001

    Article Google Scholar

  • Swami V (2011) Marked for life? A prospective study of tattoos on appearance anxiety and dissatisfaction, perceptions of uniqueness, and self-esteem. Body Image 8(3):237–244. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2011.04.005

    Article Google Scholar

  • Swami V (2012) Written on the body? Individual differences between British adults who do and do not obtain a first tattoo. Scand J Psychol 53(5):407–412

    Article Google Scholar

  • Swami V, Gaughan H, Tran US, Kuhlmann T, Stieger S, Voracek M (2015) Are tattooed adults really more aggressive and rebellious than those without tattoos? Body Image 15:149–152. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2015.09.001

    Article Google Scholar

  • Swami V, Pietschnig J, Bertl B, Nader IW, Stieger S, Voracek M (2012) Personality differences between tattooed and non-tattooed individuals. Psychol Reports 111(1):97–106. https://doi.org/10.2466/09.07.21.Pr0.111.4.97-106

    Article Google Scholar

  • Swami V, Tran US, Kuhlmann T, Stieger S, Gaughan H, Voracek M (2016) More similar than different: tattooed adults are only slightly more impulsive and willing to take risks than Non-tattooed adults. Pers Individ Differ 88:40–44. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.08.054

    Article Google Scholar

  • Tate JC, Shelton BL (2008) Personality correlates of tattooing and body piercing in a college sample: the kids are alright. Pers Individ Differ 45(4):281–285. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2008.04.011

    Article Google Scholar

  • Taylor AJ (1974) Criminal tattoos. Int Rev Appl Psychol 23(2):121–130. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1464-0597.1974.tb00313.x

    Article Google Scholar

  • Tews MJ, Stafford K, Kudler EP (2020) The influence of tattoo content on perceptions of employment suitability across the generational divide. J Pers Psychol 19(1):4–13. https://doi.org/10.1027/1866-5888/a000234

    Article Google Scholar

  • Thielgen MM, Schade S, Rohr J (2020) How criminal offenders perceive police officers’ appearance: effects of uniforms and tattoos on inmates’ attitudes. J Forens Psychol Res Pract 20(3):214–240. https://doi.org/10.1080/24732850.2020.1714408

    Article Google Scholar

  • Thomas JN, Copulsky D (2021) Diversifying conceptions of sexual pleasure in self-reported genital piercing stories. Deviant Behav 42(8):1032–1042. https://doi.org/10.1080/01639625.2020.1718834

    Article Google Scholar

  • Thompson K (2015) Comparing the psychosocial health of tattooed and non-tattooed women. Pers Individ Differ 74:122–126. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.10.010

    Article Google Scholar

  • Tiggemann M, Golder F (2006) Tattooing: an expression of uniqueness in the appearance domain. Body Image 3(4):309–315. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2006.09.002

    Article Google Scholar

  • Tiggemann M, Hopkins LA (2011) Tattoos and piercings: bodily expressions of uniqueness? Body Image 8(3):245–250

    Article Google Scholar

  • Timming A, Nickson D, Re D, Perrett D (2017) What do you think of my ink? Assessing the effects of body art on employment chances. Human Resour Manag: published in cooperation with the School of Business Administration, The University of Michigan and in alliance with the Society of Human Resources Management 56(1):133–149. https://doi.org/10.1002/hrm.21770

    Article Google Scholar

  • Timming AR (2015) Visible tattoos in the service sector: a new challenge to recruitment and selection. Work Employm Society 29(1):60–78

    Article Google Scholar

  • Timming AR (2017) Body art as branded labour: at the intersection of employee selection and relationship marketing. Hum Relat 70(9):1041

    Article Google Scholar

  • Timming AR, Perrett D (2016) Trust and mixed signals: a study of religion, tattoos and cognitive dissonance. Pers Individ Differ 97:234–238. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.03.067

    Article Google Scholar

  • Vale V, Juno A (eds.) (1989). Modern primitives: an investigation of contemporary adornment and ritual. RE/Search Publications

  • Vizgaitis AL, Lenzenweger MF (2019) Pierced identities: body modification, borderline personality features, identity, and self-concept disturbances. Pers Disord Theor Res Treatment 10(2):154

    Article Google Scholar

  • Wasarhaley NE, Vilk RF (2020) More than skin deep? The effect of visible tattoos on the perceived characteristics of a rape victim. Women Crim Just 30(2):106–125

    Article Google Scholar

  • Willman JC, Hernando R, Matu M, Crevecoeur I (2020) Biocultural diversity in Late Pleistocene/Early Holocene Africa: Olduvai Hominid 1 (Tanzania) biological affinity and intentional body modification. Am J Phy Anthropol 172(4):664–681. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.24007

    Article Google Scholar

  • Wiseman DB (2010a) Patient characteristics that impact tattoo removal resource‐allocation choices: tattoo content, age, and parental status. J Appl Soc Psychol 40(11):2736–2749

    Article Google Scholar

  • Wiseman DB (2010b) Perceptions of a tattooed college instructor. Psychol Rep 106(3):845–850

    Article Google Scholar

  • Wohlrab S, Fink B, Kappeler PM, Brewer G (2009a) Differences in personality attributions toward tattooed and nontattooed virtual human characters. J Individ Differ 30(1):1. https://doi.org/10.1027/1614-0001.30.1.1

    Article Google Scholar

  • Wohlrab S, Fink B, Kappeler PM, Brewer G (2009b) Perception of human body modification. Pers Individ Differ 46(2):202. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2008.09.031

    Article Google Scholar

  • Zestcott CA, Bean MG, Stone J (2017) Evidence of negative implicit attitudes toward individuals with a tattoo near the face. Group Process Intergroup Relat 20(2):186–201

    Article Google Scholar

  • Zestcott CA, Stone J (2019) Experimental evidence that a patient’s tattoo increases their assigned health care cost liability. Stigma Health 4(4):442

    Article Google Scholar

  • Zestcott CA, Tompkins TL, Kozak Williams M, Livesay K, Chan KL (2018) What do you think about ink? An examination of implicit and explicit attitudes toward tattooed individuals. J Soc Psychol 158(1):7

    Article Google Scholar

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

  1. Department of Psychology, University of Sunderland, Sunderland, SR13SD, UK

    Rebecca Owens

  2. Department of Anthropology, Temple University, Philadelphia, PA, 19122, USA

    Steven J. Filoromo

  3. Department of Anthropology, University of Alabama, Tuscaloosa, AL, 35487, USA

    Steven J. Filoromo,Christopher D. Lynn&Michael R. A. Smetana

  4. Department of Philosophy, University of Alabama, Tuscaloosa, AL, 35487, USA

    Lauren A. Landgraf

Authors

  1. Rebecca Owens

    You can also search for this author in PubMedGoogle Scholar

  2. Steven J. Filoromo

    You can also search for this author in PubMedGoogle Scholar

  3. Lauren A. Landgraf

    You can also search for this author in PubMedGoogle Scholar

  4. Christopher D. Lynn

    You can also search for this author in PubMedGoogle Scholar

  5. Michael R. A. Smetana

    You can also search for this author in PubMedGoogle Scholar

Contributions

RO: conceptualisation, data curation, methodology, visualisation, writing (original draft and editing). MS, LL, and SF: data curation, formal analysis, writing (review & editing). CL: Project administration, formal analysis, supervision, writing (review & editing).

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Rebecca Owens.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Ethical approval

This article does not contain any studies with human participants performed by any of the authors.

Informed consent

This research did not require informed consent as it is a review paper.

Additional information

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Deviance as an historical artefact: a scoping review of psychological studies of body modification (2)

Cite this article

Owens, R., Filoromo, S.J., Landgraf, L.A. et al. Deviance as an historical artefact: a scoping review of psychological studies of body modification. Humanit Soc Sci Commun 10, 33 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-023-01511-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-023-01511-6

Deviance as an historical artefact: a scoping review of psychological studies of body modification (2024)

FAQs

How is deviance explained in the psychological perspective? ›

In general, in the psychological sciences, deviant behavior is usually referred to a set of actions or individual actions that violate social and psychological, moral norms, rules and principles adopted in the society; behavior that causes psychological, physical, social and moral damage to both the individual and the ...

Is body modification a deviant behavior? ›

However, the psychological motivations for body modification practices are still being investigated regarding deviance or risky behaviours, contributing to a sense in the academic literature that body modifications are both normal and deviant.

What is the theory of deviance in psychology? ›

Deviance is a sociological concept referring to behaviors that violate social rules and norms. People who have substance use disorders are often described as being socially deviant because they do not meet society's expectations for what is considered acceptable behavior.

What is an example of deviance in psychology? ›

A deviant behavior in society is one that goes against social norms or laws established by society. Examples of deviant behavior in society may include alcoholism, underage drinking, eating disorders, and sex exploitation.

What are the examples of body modification? ›

Dieting, body-building, tanning, ear piercing and cosmetic surgery have long been common in the United States, and practices such as tattooing, body piercing and scarification are becoming increasingly popular.

What makes a behavior psychologically deviant? ›

Deviant behavior is any behavior that is contrary to the dominant norms of society. There are many different theories on what causes a person to perform deviant behavior, including biological explanations, sociological explanations, as well as psychological explanations.

What are the causes of body modification? ›

Body modifications may be done for cosmetic or reconstructive reasons. People who have cosmetic procedures typically are unhappy with the size, shape, color or texture of some body part and choose to have it changed in some way. Others may need procedures to reconstruct injured or damaged body parts or areas.

What is deviance in psychological disorder? ›

Social deviance refers to actions or behaviours that violate social norms. Since the declassification of hom*osexuality and development of DSM-III, one of the aims of a definition of mental disorder has been to make explicit the distinction between mental disorder and social deviance.

What are the psychological explanations of crime and deviance? ›

There are four basic aspects of psychological theories of crime, which say that crime is a result of failures in psychological development, learned behaviors of aggression and violence, inherent personality traits, and the relationship of criminality to mental illness.

How does psychoanalytic theory explain deviance? ›

Answer and Explanation:

Freud believed that psychological dysfunction, or deviance, was caused by too much repression. In this context, the ego tries to subconsciously keep anxieties and impulses hidden away so that we aren't aware of them.

What do some psychologists focus on to explain social deviance? ›

Social-psychological research into deviance has focused primarily on the way that individual deviants respond to group pressure and the way that groups respond to individual members who deviate from the group norms.

Top Articles
Latest Posts
Article information

Author: Nathanial Hackett

Last Updated:

Views: 6231

Rating: 4.1 / 5 (52 voted)

Reviews: 83% of readers found this page helpful

Author information

Name: Nathanial Hackett

Birthday: 1997-10-09

Address: Apt. 935 264 Abshire Canyon, South Nerissachester, NM 01800

Phone: +9752624861224

Job: Forward Technology Assistant

Hobby: Listening to music, Shopping, Vacation, Baton twirling, Flower arranging, Blacksmithing, Do it yourself

Introduction: My name is Nathanial Hackett, I am a lovely, curious, smiling, lively, thoughtful, courageous, lively person who loves writing and wants to share my knowledge and understanding with you.